Metodološkizvezki,Vol. 14,No. 1,2017,1–17 SurveyModeasaModeratorofContextEffects MayDoušak 1 Abstract After decades of survey quality research and many experiments on the mode of data collection as a source of data inequivalence, there is still no exact explanation of how the mixed-mode design impacts the collected data. While not providing a definitive answer, we propose a new explanation of the mode effect and how it cor- relates with other sources of errors in surveys. By reviewing the relevant literature, we develop theoretical arguments indicating that the mode of data collection is in factamoderatorofcontexteffects,aviewthatcanprovideananswertothequestion concerningtheoriginofthedatacacophonyinmixed-modecomparativestudies. 1 Introduction Research on the comparability of data obtained using different data collection modes in surveys is often associated with recent developments in quantitative social research. The falling response rates in traditional self-administered paper, face-to-face and telephone surveys together with the ever greater time and budget constraints create challenges for thetraditionalsurveyapproach. Asaresultandgiventhemanypossibilitiesdigitalisation offers, it is not uncommon for traditional data collection modes in both longitudinal and cross-sectionalstudiestobecomplementedwithdatacollectedthroughtheInternet. After all, this is a way of reducing both the cost and duration of the survey fieldwork process (DillmanandChristian,2005;Jäckle,Roberts,andLynn,2010). Yet, in reality, these are not new developments. Market research agencies have been mixingmodesofdatacollectionsincethe 1930safter discovering thattelephonesurveys provide a faster and more cost-effective way of obtaining data in larger American towns (Karol, 1937). In 1944, Deming factored “Different kinds and degrees of canvas” as one of the sources of errors in surveys. He also noted that “cheaper methods” such as telephone (nowadays the Web) may hold greater utility once the comparability of data collectionmodesisobtainedthroughmoreextensiveresearch(Deming,1944). Regarding research on the comparability of data obtained using different modes, already in 1963 Cannell and Fowler developed a set of hypotheses on how the interviewer’s presence affectsarespondent’smotivationandontheunderreportingofpotentially“embarrassing” data(CannellandFowler,1963). Seventy years after Deming’s article, great progress has been made on systematis- ing measurement errors in different data collection modes (de Leeuw and Berzelak in Wolfetal,2016;Berzelak,2014;Vannieuwunhuyze,Loosveldt,andMolenberghs,2010; 1 FacultyofSocialSciences,UniversityofLjubljana,Ljubljana,Slovenia;may.dousak@fdv.uni-lj.si 2 M. Doušak Roberts,2007),althoughthereisstillageneralconsensusamongtheresearchcommunity that the different modes do not provide equivalent data and therefore combining modes shouldbeavoidedwherepossible(MartinandLynn,2011). 2 In addition, the advent of connected portable devices such as smartphones and wear- ables that store vast amounts of users’ information online and give researchers an op- portunity for continuous “big data” collection raise questions about the need for survey modeswhichwouldpresentanalternativetocostlyandlengthyface-to-faceorlatelyeven mailed paper and pencil questionnaires (Mayer-Schönberger and Cukier, 2013). Com- binedwithtimeandbudgetconstraints,respondents’reluctancetoparticipatealongwith greatercompetivenessfromprivatemarketingandsurveyagencies,thisforcesresearchers intostreamliningsurveydesignsandseekingnewapproachestocollectingdata. This explains why, in practice, the choice of a combination of data collection modes or new approaches is sometimes all that is available to researchers in order to continue with their research. To ensure high quality data, researchers must carefully assess all possible measurement specifics of different data collection modes and sources of bias (Jäckle,Roberts,andLynn,2010). Forthispurpose,weproposeanewviewontheabove phenomenon. Based on evidence stemming from both theoretical and empirical sources, weconceptuallydiscussthethesisthatthemodeofdatacollectionmoderatesthecontext effects. 2 AboutContextEffects Before digging deeper into mode effects and their relationship with context effects, we considerdifferentauthors’viewsoncontexteffectsbysummarisingcertainkeyviewson contexteffectsfoundintheliterature. 3 Social methodologists typically refer to them as intra-survey stimuli that can have a noticeable effect on the measurement (Schwarz and Strack, 1991). The most evident is the context given in the question wording: asking about one’s attitude to abortion in the context of saving a mother’s life might bring different responses to the same question in thecontextofunwantedchildren(TourangeauandRasinski,1988). In addition to this more obvious example, various experiments show that prior ques- tions or topics can also influence responses to later questions (a phenomenon called the question-order effect) (Hafner-Fink and Uhan, 2013). Tourangeau and Rasinski’s (1988) four-stage response process in attitude measurement clearly explains this bias caused by priortopicsinasurvey. Accordingtotheirdefinition,respondents: 1. “firstinterprettheattitudequestion,determiningwhatattitudethequestionisabout”, 2. “retrieverelevantbeliefsandfeelings”, 3. “applythesebeliefsandfeelingsinrenderingtheappropriatejudgement” 2 While this is true for most surveys, researchers sometimes combine the interviewer-administered data collection mode (e.g. CAPI) with the self-administered mode for part of the questionnaire (e.g. drop-off questionnaire, CASI) for sensitive topics where the interviewer’s involvement might have an influence on therespondent(deLeeuw,2005). 3 A discussion of all of the context effects, their causes and consequences lies beyond the scope of this article;weonlyrefertosomeofthecontexteffectsneededtoconveyourmessage. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 3 4. “usethisjudgementtoselectaresponse”(TourangeauandRasinski,1988: 300). Ifthe“attitude”andthusalso“relevantbeliefsandfeelings”havealreadybeendeter- mined and retrieved by prior items in a survey, they could be retrieved in the context of something which may have a great influence on their judgement process. Tourangeau et al. (2000) later expanded the four-stage response process beyond attitude measurement only, although they still discussed the attitudinal questions in a separate section due to their greater measurement instability. There they explicitly noted “minor question word- ingororder”(Tourangeauetal.,2000:13)asoneofthereasonsforshiftsinmeasurement. Whilethereisnoreasonnottoapplysuchideastofactualtypesofquestions,herewelimit thediscussionsolelytoattitudinalquestions. The example of a question on one’s attitude to abortion could also have an impact on more general questions later in the survey as the term “abortion” has already been put into context. Schuman and Presser did a split-ballot experiment on this very topic and demonstrated a 13% point difference in answering “yes” to the same question “Do you thinkitshouldbepossibleforapregnantwomantoobtainalegalabortionifsheismarried and does not want any more children?” due to the question-order effect (Schuman and Presser,1996). Holyk also explained context effect in terms of priming as psychologists refer to it (Holyk in Lavrakas, 2008). By priming, the words that are semantically or associatively relatedtopriortermsinaquestionnairearerecognisedfasterthanotherwords. Togivean example, if “doctor” is mentioned in previous questions, the word “nurse” is recognised faster. In this case, interpretation and determination of what the question is about occur more impulsively and may be given less thought and judgement. For some authors, es- pecially social methodologists, context effects come only from within the questionnaire itself (intra-survey stimuli). Besides question wording, question order and priming men- tionedabove,intra-surveystimuliinclude(butarenotlimitedto)responsescaleselection, serialpositioneffect(primacy,recency),andothers(Lavrakas,2008). In contrast, Bradburn (1991) pointed out that context effects consist of much more than just question-order effects or even the questionnaire itself. He argued that “every- thing that occurs during an interview has the potential to influence the answers given by respondents to the questions asked by the interviewer” – a view that also accounts for extra-survey stimuli (Bradburn, 1991). While some readers might argue that his choice of wording implies the presence of the interviewer, the author also discusses mail ques- tionnaires. By explicitly mentioning nonsemantic information during an interview such as “the social context within which the interview takes place”, Bradburn takes a broader, psychologist’s viewpoint on the survey context. Due to such different views on the con- textofsurveyinterviews,SchwarzandSudmanevencontendedthatcognitiveresearchers andsocialmethodologistsdonotspeakthesamelanguage(SchwarzandSudman,1991). Giventhatthemostcommonlyrecognisedmodelforthecognitiveprocessesunderly- ingasurveyconversation(Tourangeau,RipsandRasinski,2000)aswellasitsdescendant model of satisficing (Krosnick, 1991) both come from psychologists, we suggest taking a wider view of context effects in surveys, similarly to Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda and Vehovar(2015). Underthisbroaderview,stimulicanalsocomefromoutsidethesurveyquestionnaire itself (extra-survey context effects). Examples of such stimuli include (among others) 4 M. Doušak time constraint and external distractions such as looking out for children or driving a car while responding to an interview as the respondent is not completely engaged in the sur- vey response process. According to this view, even the current political situation and the respondent’sculturalbackgroundmayhaveaninfluenceontheirresponses. Thepresence ofotherpeoplecanalsoconsiderablyinfluenceresponsestoquestionsonsensitivetopics –imagineaskingateenagerabouttheirexperiencewithmarijuanainthepresenceoftheir parents. When talking about the presence of other people, social conformity should be mentioned(Kelman,1958). Askingaboutone’sattitudetoabortionataChristiangather- ingwouldseeresultsaspredicted. Eventheinterviewer’spresencecanimpactresponses suchthattherespondentover-reportssociallyacceptablebehaviourandunder-reportsless socially acceptable behaviour, what is called “social desirability bias” (Roxas and Lind- say,2012;ChangandKrosnick,2010;Sakshaug,Yan,andTourangeau,2010;Tourangeau and Yan, 2007). In order to reduce the cognitive burden of providing accurate and con- sistent answers, the respondent might also answer in terms of satisficing the interviewer (Krosnick,1999). Theinterviewer,specifically,canalsoberegardedasasourceofcontexteffect. Name- ly, an important aspect of the interviewer’s presence is their conversational implicatures 4 (Grice, 1974). Without an interviewer, the respondent hears (pre-recorded) or only reads the question literally as it is written. While the interviewer should read the questions literally as they are written in the questionnaire, he or she does indeed have an influence on the flow of conversation. Grice (1974) states that dialogues should comply with four maxims: • quantity(beasinformativeasrequired–butnotmore) • quality(donotspreaduntrueorunconfirmedinformation) • relation(berelevant) • manner (it is not about the content but about the way the information is given in termsofambiguities,obscurities,prolixityetc.) (Grice,1974,p. 47). According to these four maxims, the conversation can indeed change when an inter- viewer is present compared to self-interviewing (the so-called interviewer effect). Apart fromsemanticcommunication,thesimilarlyimportantnonsemantic,tacitcommunication between the respondent and the interviewer should not be disregarded. Both Uhan and Bradburn used Grice’s framework to argue that interviewer-respondent communication canbeseenasasourceofcontexteffects(Bradburn,1991;Uhan,1998). Anothersourceofcontexteffectcanbestimulithatcaninfluencearesponseifsome- onedoesnothavefirst-handexperienceonthetopic–thisfactisheavilyrelieduponinthe media, marketing and politics to attract customers or win over sympathisers. One exam- ple is an individual’s attitude to certain groups of people (based on religion, nationality, sexualorientation,sportclubloyaltyorothercharacteristics)theyhavenevermetbutare givenanegativeconnotationinmediareadbytherespondent. Uhan(1998)goesevenfurtherwithhissystematicdistinctionof“local”and“global” contexts. The local context encompasses the survey questionnaire and survey situation 4 Indirectly,implicitlyconveyedmeaninginspeech. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 5 which not only consist of wording and question-order effects, but also scale selection, content validity, the respondent’s acquiescence, 5 standardisation of procedures and rel- evant underlying psychological processes and respondent capability. The latter hints at both cognitive processes underlying survey interviewing (Tourangeau, Rips, and Rasin- ski, 2000) as well as the satisficing model (Krosnick, 1999). The idea of local context encompassingmuchmorethanthequestionnaireitselfisalsosupportedbyotherauthors whorecognisethecognitiveprocessmodelascapableofexplaining“measurementerrors duetorespondents”(Callegaro,LozarManfreda,andVehovar,2015,p. 64). The global context is defined as the specific environment of the interview, formed by the cultural as well as personality traits of both the interviewer and the respondent who define the interview’s properties through the constellation of their relationship (Uhan, 1998). The author expands Bingham and More’s (1934) view who state that a survey interview is a “conversation with a purpose”, and Bradburn’s (1991) view on context (the “social context within which the interview takes place”) by suggesting that a survey interviewisaspecificsocialevent. This definition is particularly valuable while assessing the comparability of survey data between multiple global contexts among different cultural environments (cross-na- tional surveys) or different target groups of respondents. Moreover, the global context hypothesis can also explain differences in data obtained from different data collection modes such as the interviewer effect as the interviewer brings his/her beliefs, personal traits,experienceandknowledgeintoalloftheinterviewshe/sheconducts(Uhan,1998). While one might argue that conversational implicatures should fall within the local con- text, Uhan explicitly placed them in the global context to emphasise the traits both the respondentandtheinterviewerbringintothesurveysituation. Respondent motivation is another aspect of the context within which a survey inter- view is conducted. A respondent can be motivated by either the content of the survey (a closeorsalienttopic)oritspresentation(bytheinterviewer,visualappearance,technical form of the presentation) (Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda, and Vehovar, 2015; Dillman and Christian,2005). Basedontheabovediscussion,wesystemisesomeoftheselectedcon- texteffectsbypositioningthemwithintwocategorisationsarisingfromdifferentauthors’ views: theviewofthelocalandglobalcontexteffectsversustheviewofintra-surveyand extra-surveystimuli,asshowninTable1. In summary, we see context effects as arising not only from the survey questionnaire itself, but also from the survey situation, constellation of the interviewer and respon- dent,aswellastheircultural,politicalandsocialbackgrounds. Thisbroadinterpretation, whichencompassesviewsheldbybothsocialmethodologistsandpsychologists,provides the most suitable basis for our main argument, namely that survey mode moderates the contexteffect. Toarguetherelationshipbetweenthetwo,wewillmorespecificallyfocus onextra-surveystimulistemmingfrommode-inherentcharacteristics. 5 Acquiescence can be defined as a personality trait (Couch and Keniston, 1960), as well as an effect of bothsurveycontent(Adorno,etal. 1950;SchumanandPresser,1996)andsurveysituation(socialcontext) (Campbell,etal.,1960). WhileUhan(1998)explicitlyplaceditinlocalcontextinconnectiontothesurvey instrument, we refer to it in both the local and global contexts due to the different views on its cause and nature. Thephenomenonhasbeenstudiedthoroughlyinpsychologicalliteraturewhichtheinterestedreader mayconsult(DeMaio,1985;Adornoetal.,1950;Campbell,etal.,1960;Uhan,1998). 6 M. Doušak Table1: Systematisationofglobalandlocalcontexteffectsversusintra-surveyand extra-surveystimuli Intra-surveystimuli Extra-surveystimuli Localcontext • Questionwording • Questionorder,priming • Serial position effect (recency, primacy) • Acquiescence • Respondentmotivation • Scaleselection • Standardisationofprocedures • Timeconstraint • Externaldistractions • Socialconformity • Acquiescence • Respondentmotivation • Satisficing • Respondentcapability • Social context during inter- view Globalcontext • Acquiescence • Interviewer’s personality traits andbeliefs • Conversational implicatures/ interviewereffect • Respondent’spersonalitytraits andbeliefs • Cultural background (of re- spondentandinterviewer) • Politicalandsocialcontext Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 7 3 Mode Effects and their Relationship with Context Ef- fects Themodesemployedincollectingdatainthemeasurementstageareconstantlyevolving, from paper and pencil, first documented in 1788, through face to face (1912), telephone (1930s)andmorerecentlymanyothermodestowebandmobile(Callegaro,LozarMan- freda, and Vehovar, 2015; de Heer, de Leeuw, and van der Zouwen, 1999; de Leeuw, 1992). Notonlyaretheyconstantlyevolving,mostdatacollectionmodesareheretostay sincenoneoutperformstheothersinallrespects(Couper,2011). Theycanalsobemixed in either a longitudinal or cross-sectional way, that is, when one mode is superseded by anotherinalong-runningcomparativesurvey,orwhenmultipledatacollectionmodesare usedsimultaneously,suchasbytheInternationalSocialSurveyProgramme(ISSP;ISSP, 2010). Theproblemresearchersfaceisthatthedifferentmodesofdatacollectionsometimes provide unequal data due to their specific mode-inherent characteristics. This data in- equityphenomenoniscalledthemodeeffect. Abundantresearchandliteratureisavailable for the interested reader to consult 6 on the modes of data collection, their main charac- teristics and the problem of combining them; however, the research on mode effects is lackinginitsconclusions(deLeeuw,2005). For the purpose of this article, which seeks to relate mode effects to context effects, we shall explain the mode effect by narrowing our focus to some of the most commonly identified mode-inherent characteristics. But before that, let us raise attention to some effects which are commonly mistaken for mode effects and should be disregarded in our discussion. Whenmixingdatacollectionmodes,researcherssometimesgiverespondentsthefree- dom to choose their own preferred mode for participation. Self-administered paper-and- pencil questionnaires can be combined with the Web by writing the URL of the Web questionnaireontopofpage1ofthequestionnaire(LozarManfredaandVehovar,2002). Asrespondents select themodeoftheirparticipationbasedontheirpreferences, anydif- ferences between the modes in the resulting data should not be attributed to the mode effect only but to the selection effect as well. Differences in coverage and nonresponse of different modes also should not be mistaken for the effect of the data collection mode perse(Sax,Gilmartin,andBryant,2003;Dillman,etal.,2009;Kaplowitz,Hadlock,and Levine,2004). Anotherexampleofeffectwhichcanhowevernotberegardedasamodeeffectrefers to the following situation. Survey design errors can remain hidden and undetected even after many years and waves of a survey. Sometimes, they are exposed by a change in the data collection mode. Dillman and Christian (2005) provide a vivid example of a survey which identified significantly more “divorced” and “widowed” and fewer “sin- gle”respondentsafterthedatacollectionmodewasswitchedfromtelephonetotheWeb. When looking into it, they found a huge shortcoming in the survey design: when the re- spondentshadbeenaskedabouttheirmaritalstatusoverthetelephone,theyweresimply 6 Examples include (among others) de Leeuw and Berzelak in Wolf et al, 2016; Callegaro, Lozar Man- freda, and Vehovar, 2015; Berzelak, 2014; Groves, et al., 2009; de Leeuw, Hox, and Dillman, 2008; Tourangeau,Rips,andRasinski,2000. 8 M. Doušak askedabouttheirstatuswithoutbeingprovidedthefulllistofoptions. Manywidowedor divorced respondents answered truthfully that they were single. In contrast, web respon- dents were given a full list of possible answers so they chose one of options provided. The inequivalence of the data collected in such cases is not mode-inherent as it could be avoided in the survey design (disregarding the recency effect we will mention later, the telephonequestioncouldbereformulatedto“NowIwillreadsomeofthemaritalstatuses (...) Pleasechoosetheonethatisnearesttoyourcurrentmaritalstatus”). The above examples are not really mode-inherent effects and can (and therefore sho- uld) be reduced by the survey design (Hox, de Leeuw, and Zijlmans, 2015; Berzelak, 2014; de Leeuw, Hox, and Dillman, 2008). Thus, some of the most obvious causes of data inequivalence stemming from different data collection modes can be ruled out if theyarereducedbythesurveydesign. Whichthenaremode-inherentcharacteristicsthat createthemodeeffectandcannotbeavoidedevenwithacleversurveydesign? Whiledifferentauthorsdefinemode-inherentcharacteristicsandhencedifferencesbe- tweenthemodesintheirownterms,mode-specificcharacteristicsmaybroadlybedefined asacombinationoftheinterviewer’sinvolvement,thedegreeofcontactwiththerespon- dent, the channel of communication, the media of communication, the locus of control, degree of privacy and the presence of computer technology 7 (Berzelak, 2014; Couper, 2011; Groves et al. 2009; de Leeuw, 1992). Authors list such characteristics as mode- inherent properties that create mode effects and usually cannot be avoided by a clever survey design or sampling (Berzelak, 2014). While this article cannot fully review all of them or provide an overall single view on them, we shall briefly examine four of the characteristics we believe are typically given more thought when designing survey 8 and relateeachofthemtocontexteffects. Involvement of the interviewer is certainly the most obvious and generally agreed mode characteristic (Couper, 2011; Groves, et al., 2009; de Leeuw, 1992; Tourangeau, Rips,andRasinski,2000). On the positive side, the interviewer can provide a controlled environment for the interviewbyarrangingthetimeand(withaface-to-facesurvey)locationoftheinterview so it can be conducted with the fewest interruptions possible — in contextual terms, he can ensure the most appropriate social context for the interview (Bradburn, 1991). By providingthemostappropriatesocialcontext,theinterviewercanconsequentlyinfluence thelocalcontextwithinwhichtheinterviewiscarriedout. The interviewer also relieves the respondent of the cognition burden induced by the questionnaire by taking the locus of control into his domain (for example, by taking care oftheroutinginthequestionnaire)oreventalkingtolessliteraterespondentswhowould struggletoreadthequestionnaire. Trainedinterviewerscanalsomotivateandengagethe respondent (Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda, and Vehovar, 2015). Both respondent capability andrespondentmotivationarementionedintheprevioussectionandfallwithinthelocal contextasdefinedbyUhan(1998). Consequently,themodeeffectduetotheinterviewer’s presencecanmoderatethecontextregardingrespondentscapabilityandmotivationwhen understoodintermsofBradburn(1991)andUhan(1998). Despite being a professional, an interviewer can sometimes influence a respondent 7 Foramorein-depthoverviewofmodecharacteristics,seeBerzelak(2014),pp. 32–36. 8 E.g. bydecidingontheinterviewerinvolvement,useofcomputeror/andshowcards,etc. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 9 via conversational implicatures (see the section above). This is a global context effect whichisonlypresentinsurveymodesthatuseaninterviewer. Somerespondentsfeelthe interviewer is a representative of authority (Uhan, 1998). Both the traditional and con- temporarypsychologicaltheoryofsocialinfluenceexplaintheprocessesoccurringinthe interviewer’spresence,suchascomplianceandsocialdesirability. First,Kelman’s(1958) compliance social influence process 9 can clearly be identified by the interviewer’s pres- ence. Compliancemeansarespondentprovidesaparticularanswer“toachievefavorable reaction from another person a group” (Kelman 1958, p. 3). More recently, Friedkin (1998) built upon the theory of social influence by adding a structural approach to it. In addition,TourangeauandYan(2007)notethatsocialdesirabilityisquitecommoninsur- veys (Tourangeau and Yan, 2007). Being influenced by the presence of the interviewer or a third party on one hand and the sense of privacy in self-administered data collection modesontheother,socialdesirabilityiswidelyacceptedasamode-inherentcharacteris- tic (Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda, and Vehovar, 2015; Tourangeau and Yan, 2007; Couper, 2011; Dillman and Christian, 2005). While social desirability is indeed a mode-inherent effect, Bradburn (1991) also suggests that it is part of the social context in which the surveyinterviewisconducted. Basedontheabovearguments,weproposethatinterviewerpresence(involvement)is amoderatorofthesurveycontextandthusofthecontexteffects. Thepresenceofcomputertechnologyisidentifiedasamode-inherentcharacteristic bydifferentauthors(Couper,2011;deLeeuw,2005;Berzelak,2014). Computerpresence usedtohave(andthereforecanstillhave)animpactonthedataevenwhenusedsolelyby theinterviewerbecausesomerespondentsfearthatcomputersaremonitoringtheconsis- tency of their answers (de Leeuw, 1992). While later sources disregard this phenomenon as today being almost negligible (Snijkers, Hox, and de Leeuw, 1995; de Leeuw, 2002), the presence of a computer may still change the relations and social setting, thereby af- fectingthesocialcontext. Anothercomputer-relatedeffectthatcanonlybeobservedwhenrespondentsactually have to use it is the level of acquaintance with computer technology (Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda, and Vehovar, 2015). When a computer is used in telephone surveys, only the locus of control changes from the interviewer to the computer – the respondent neither sees nor uses the device but the interviewer’s work is made easier so they can be more concentratedonmotivatingtherespondent. Consequently, we find that the computer forms part of the social setting in which the interview is carried out. When used by the respondent (compared to a paper-based questionnaire),thecomputermaytakethelocusofcontrolaswellasengageandmotivate the respondent through visual design, interactivity and other elements (Callegaro, Lozar Manfreda, and Vehovar, 2015). As the presence of computer technology influences the socialsettingwhichispartofthe[social]context,italsoinfluencesthecontexteffects. Another mode-inherent characteristic is the channel of communication (Groves, et al.,2009;Berzelak,2014). Itcanbeaural(telephone),visual(paperorwebquestionnaire) oracombinationofboth(face-to-faceinterview). Tourangeau,RipsandRasinskigoeven further and distinguish channels of presentation and responding (Tourangeau, Rips, and 9 Kelmanidentifiedthreesocialinfluenceprocesses: compliance,identificationandinternalisation(Kel- man,1958). 10 M. Doušak Rasinski, 2000). An example of an interview involving different channels of presenta- tion and responding is IVR where the respondent listens to the question and then enters the appropriate answer on the telephone keypad. The traditional literature discusses the dichotomy of oral andliteral communicationwhereby written language is more analytic, classificatoryandstructuredwhileoralisredundantandaggregative(Ong,1982). Murray (1988)systematisesthisdichotomyasemotional,personal,context-boundoralcommuni- cationvs. analyticandsequentialthatissuitableforrationalandcriticalthinkingwritten text. He (Murray, 1988) further experimented with the computer as the medium of data collectionandconcludedthatmodeandmediumrepresentchoicesmadeduetothechar- acteristics of the particular context in which the interaction takes place (Murray, 1988). Whiledemonstratingthatliteracyandoralityarenotdichotomous,asotherwisesuggested bythetraditionalliterature, heplacedchannelaswellasmediumofcommunication(de- scribed here as mode-inherent characteristics) directly in the context of the interaction (Murray,1988). In addition, de Leeuw, Hox, and Dillman (2008) suggested that social conventions, also defined as global context effects by Uhan (1998), can be shaped by the channel of communication. Mediumofcommunicationisthelastmode-inherentcharacteristicweusetodemon- strate that context effects are moderated by the mode of data collection. Primarily influ- enced by Tourangeau and de Leeuw, Berzelak defined it as an information transmission medium, which is “the tool (or service) used to transfer questionnaire and answers be- tween the respondent and the researcher” (Berzelak, 2014, p. 34). He further explains that it can be “in person, via telephone line, mail or other form of physical delivery, e- mail, or web” (Berzelak, 2014, p. 34). Returning to his sources, we find the definition of medium of communication given by de Leeuw (2005) to be one of the most complete and context-aware. She defines medium-related factors as “social conventions and cus- toms associated with the media utilized in the survey methods, such as familiarity with a medium,useofmediumandlocusofcontrol”(deLeeuw,2005,p. 244). This definition is literally written in terms of context effects by explicitly mentioning “social conventions and customs”, similarly to Bradburn (1991) and Uhan (1998). In our opinion, this definition is another formulation of the global contextual view on the interviewprocess. Asweunderstandit, shemeantthattheuseofanymediumpersecan causeacertaintracesonthedataassocialconventionsandcustomsvaryamongdifferent media of communication. Therefore, when the medium of communication is changed, theglobalcontextwithalltherelatedconventionsandcustomsalsochanges. Selected mode-inherent characteristics, their (alleged) impact on the survey data as wellastheirrelationshiptocontexteffectsasdescribedabovearesummarisedinTable2 below. Let us give an empirical example to illustrate our notion of mode being a moderator of context. It can be seen from the experience from the European Social Survey (ESS) 10 and the European Values Study (EVS): 11 in order to counteract the differences created by changing the mode from face to face (F2F) to web, in ESS they had to adapt 85% 10 European Social Survey is a repeat cross-sectional survey on attitudes, beliefs and behavior that is conductedinover30countrier(ESS,2017). 11 European Values Study is a repeated cross-section survey on basic human values that is conducted in over45countries(EVS,2017). Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 11 Table2: Systematisationofselectedmode-inherentcharacteristics,their(alleged)impacton thesurveydataandrelationshiptocontexteffects Mode- inherent characteristics Keypossibleimpactsondata Relatedcontexteffects Presence of the inter- viewer(degree of interviewer involvement) • provoke socially desirable an- swers • causeinterviewerbias • relieverespondentofcognitive burden • provide the appropriate envi- ronment • motivatetherespondent Local context: Frame the social contextoftheinterview Global context: Bring inter- viewer’spersonalitytraits,beliefs andexperienceintotheinterview Channel of communica- tion (aural vs. visual) • provide access to the illiterate (aural) provide access to the deaf(visual) • ease the survey response pro- cess for some individuals (de- pending on their characteris- tics) Local context: Trigger different conversationalimplicatures Presence of computer technology • limit(orprovide)accesstocer- taintargetgroups • instigate different social con- ventions • reduce random errors due to locusofcontrol • demonstrate social segmenta- tion (access to the computer, acquaintance) Local context: Alter the social settingduringtheinterview Global context Bias respondent’s profile by segmenting regarding access to computer and acquain- tancewiththecomputer Medium of communication a • limit(orprovide)accesstocer- taintargetgroups • triggerdifferentsocialconven- tions, reduce (increase) ran- domerrorsduetolocusofcon- trol • demonstrate social segmenta- tion(accesstothemedium) Local context: Provoke medium- specific social conventions and customs Global context Bias respondent’s profile by segmenting regarding mediumaccess a AsproposedbyBerzelak(2014)basedonTougangeauanddeLeeuw’stexts. 12 M. Doušak of the questions while in EVS they changed the wording of 156 out of 256 questions (Callegaro,LozarManfreda,andVehovar,2015)toensurethattheequivalenceofthedata was retained. We find this change in line with Ong’s (1982) dichotomy of classificatory and structured written vs. redundant and aggregative oral communication and even more so with Goody and Watt’s (1963) explanation of semantic ratification, by which “the meaning of each word is ratified in a succession of concrete situations, accompanied by vocal inflexions and physical gestures, all of which combine to particularize both its specificdenotationanditsacceptedconnotativeusages”(GoodyandWatt,1963,p. 306). BoththeESSandEVSempiricalevidenceaswellasGoodyandWatt(1963)demon- stratethatanintra-surveycontextadjustment(adaptationofthequestionnaire)canprovide (approximately) equivalent resulting data when changing the data collection mode from F2Ftoweb. AsshowninTable2,mode-inherentfeaturescanhaveaninfluenceonextra- survey context effects. Because the change of mode altered the extra-survey context and thiseffectcanbereducedbyalteringtheintra-surveycontext(questionnaire),wesuggest thatmodeitselfisamoderatorofcontextandthereforeofcontexteffects. 4 WhyitMatters The above discussion demonstrates that the mode of data collection inherently affects the context effects within which the survey interview takes place. From this viewpoint, researchers could adjust the survey design to mitigate certain local context effects while explaining some of the “difficult to predict” (Martin and Lynn, 2011, p. 30) effects in termsofUhan’sglobalcontext. Forillustrativepurpose,considertwoscenariosofcross- nationalsurveysbeingconductedinthesamecountries: 1. acompletelystandardised(coherentsampling,coordinatedtranslations,singlemode etc.) cross-nationalcomparativesurvey(suchastheEuropeanSocialSurvey);and 2. alessstandardisedcross-nationalcomparativesurvey(suchastheInternationalSo- cialSurveyProgramme). Whileglobalcontextcannotbedirectlycontrolledfor,itcanbecoherentamongsim- ilar countries that have similar respondent sampling frames and interviewer bases. How- ever,itcanalsovaryquiteconsiderablyifthecountriesinvolvedaresufficientlydifferent. In the first scenario, the completely standardised design provides the same canvas (local context effect) while the local context effect in the second example may change due to inequivalent translations, the mode change etc. In the second scenario, the countries in- volved are quite similar (very similar global context) while the local context is different duetothelessrigoroussurveydesignstandard. BothlocalandglobalcontextsaswellasthetwoscenariosarevisualisedinPicture1. Iftheglobalcontextsvarysubstantially 12 (suchasinscenario1inFigure1),providing thesamelocalcontextmaybefutilesinceirrespectiveofhowsimilarthelocalcontextis, the results are considerably inequivalent due to the different global contexts. Scenario 2 demonstratestheinversesituation: verysimilarglobalcontextsprovidethefoundationfor 12 E.g.: ifthecountriesinvolvedhavedifferentcultural,religiousorpoliticalvaluesystems. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 13 Figure1: Comparisonofthetwosurveyscenarioswiththesamelocalorsameglobal contexteffects higherdataequivalencebuttheopportunityismissedduetodifferentlocalcontextintro- duced by the less rigorous survey design specifications. The two cases produce different measuredvalues. The depiction of the two scenarios emphasizes the true importance of the survey de- sign for the data obtained. While the amount and impact of both contexts cannot be assessed beforehand without experimentation, based on the two above scenarios we ar- gue that, when the global context has a substantially greater effect on the data than the local context (which includes mode), any effort towards rigid standardization such as in scenario1maybefutile. Asthisviewcanonlybevalidwhenmodeofdatacollectionisamoderatorofcontext, wefindthisrelationshipimportant. 5 Conclusion Based on both empirical and theoretical evidence, we demonstrated that the effects of data collection mode may not be separate phenomena but an integral moderating part of context effects. This view could provide the basis for new and more effective techniques to be developed to ensure data comparability in mixed-mode comparative surveys and optimaluseofresourceswheresomestandardisationprocedureswouldproveineffective. There are many questions in need of answers so further empirical evidence and ex- perimentationisrequiredinordertoassessthedegreeofmoderationandtherelationship between different modes within standardised contexts before such techniques can be de- veloped. Until then, we should take Martin and Lynn (2011) into account who urged caution 14 M. Doušak whencomparingdataobtainedusingdifferentmodes. References [1] Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., and Sanford, R. (1950): The Au- thoritarianPersonality.NewYork: Harper. [2] Berzelak,J.(2014): ModeEffectsinWebSurveys.Ph.DthesisofUniversityofLjubl- jana,Ljubljana. [3] Bingham,W.,andMoore,B.(1934): HowtoInterview.NewYork: HarperandRow. [4] Bradburn, N. M. (1991): What have we learned? In N. Schwarz, and S. Sudman (Eds.): Context Effects in Social and Psychological Research, 315–323. New York: Springer-Verlag, [5] Callegaro, M., LozarManfreda, K., andVehovar, V.(2015): Web Survey Methodol- ogy.London: Sage. [6] Campbell,A.,Converse,P.,Miller,W.,andStokes,D.(1960): TheAmericanVoter. NewYork: Wiley. [7] Cannell,C.andFowler,F.(1963): Comparisonofaself-enumerativeprocedureand apersonalinterview: Avaliditystudy.PublicOpinionQuarterly,27(2),250–264. [8] Chang, L. and Krosnick, J. (2010): Comparing oral interviewing with self- administered computerized questionnaires: An experiment. Public Opinion Quar- terly,74(1),154–167. [9] Couch, A. and Keniston, K. (1960): Yeasayers and naysayers: Agreeing response setasapersonalityvariable.TheJournalofAbnormalandSocialPsychology,60(2), 151–174 [10] Couper, M. (2011): The future of modes of data collection. Public Opinion Quar- terly,75(5),889–908. [11] de Heer, W., de Leeuw, E. D., and van der Zouwen, J. (1999): Metholological issues in survey research: A historical review. Bulletin of Sociological Methodol- ogy/BulletindeMéthodologieSociologique,64(1),25–48. [12] de Leeuw, E. (1992): Data Quality in Mail, Telephone and Face-to-Face Surveys. Amsterdam: TT-Publikaties. [13] de Leeuw, E. (2002): The effect of computer-assisted interviewing on data quality: A review of the evidence. In J. Blasius, J. Hox, E. D. de Leeuw, and P. Schmidt (Eds.): Social Science Methodology in the New Millennium. Opladen: Leske + Bu- drich. [14] deLeeuw,E.(2005): Tomixornottomixdatacollectionmodesinsurveys.Journal ofOfficialStatistics,21(2),233–255. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 15 [15] de Leeuw, E. and Berzelak, N. (2016): Survey mode or survey modes? In C. Wolf, D.Joye,T.W.SmithandY.Fu(Eds.): TheSAGEHandbookofSurveyMethodology, 142–156.London: Sage [16] de Leeuw, E., Hox, J., and Dillman, D. (2008): International Handbook of Survey Methodology.NewYork: TaylorandFrancisGroup. [17] DeMaio, T. J. (1985): Social desirability and survey measurement: A review. In C. Turner, and E. Martin (Eds.): Surveying Subjective Phenomena 2, 257–282. New York: Sage. [18] Deming,W.(1944): Onerrorsinsurveys.AmericanSociologicalReview,9(4),359– 369. [19] Dillman, D., and Christian, L. (2005): Survey mode as a source of instability in responsesacrosssurveys.FieldMethods,17(1),30–52. [20] Dillman,D.,Phelps,G.,Tortora,R.,Swift,K.,Kohrell,J.,Berck,J.,andMesser,B. (2009): Response rate and measurement differences in mixed mode surveys using mail, telephone, Interactive Voice Response (IVR) and the Internet. Social Science Research,1–18. [21] ESS. (2017): About the European Social Survey European Research Infrastruc- ture – ESS ERIC. Downloaded 04. Sep 2017 from European Social Survey: http://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/about/ [22] EVS.(2017): AboutEVS.Downloaded04.Sep2017fromEuropeanValuesStudy: http://www.europeanvaluesstudy.eu/page/about-evs.html [23] Friedkin, N. E. (1998): A Structural Theory of Social Influence. Cambridge: Cam- bridgeUniversityPress. [24] Goody, J., Watt I. (1963): The consequences of literacy. Comparative Studies in SocietyandHistory.5(3),304–345 [25] Grice,H.(1974): Logicandconversation.InP.Cole,andL.Morgan(Eds.): Syntax andSemantics3: SpeechArts,44–58.NewYork: Elsevier. [26] Groves, R., Fowler, F., Couper, M., Lepkowski, J., Singer, E., and Tourangeau, R. (2009): SurveyMethodology.Hoboken,NJ:JohnWileyandSons. [27] Hafner-Fink, M., and Uhan, S. (2013): Bipolarity and/or duality of social survey measurement scales and the question-order effect. Quality & Quantity, 47(2), 839– 852. [28] Hox,J.,deLeeuw,E.,andZijlmans,E.(2015): Measurementequivalenceinmixed modesurveys.FrontiersinPsychology,6,1–11. [29] ISSP. (2010): General information. Downloaded 28. Sep 2016 from International SocialSurveyProgramme: http://www.issp.org/index.php 16 M. Doušak [30] Jäckle, A., Roberts, C., and Lynn, P. (2010): Assessing the effect of data collection modeonmeasurement.InternationalStatisticalReview,78(1),3–20. [31] Kaplowitz,M.,Hadlock,T.,andLevine,R.(2004): AcomparisonofWebandmail responserates.PublicOpinionQuarterly,68(1),94–101. [32] Karol, J. (1937): Measuring radio audiences. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 1(2), 92–96. [33] Krosnick, J. (1991): Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands of attitudemeasuresinsurveys.AppliedCognitivePsychology,5(3),213–236. [34] Krosnick, J. (1999): Survey research. Annual Review of Psychology, 50(1), 537– 567. [35] KelmanH.C.(1958): Compliance,identificationandinternalizationthreeprocesses ofattitudechange.ConflictResolution,2(1),51–60. [36] Lavrakas,P.(2008): EncyclopediaofSurveyResearchMethods.London: Sage. [37] Lozar Manfreda, K., and Vehovar, V. (2002): Do mail and Web surveys provide sameresults? Metodološkizvezki,18,149–169. [38] Martin, P., and Lynn, P. (2011): The Effects of Mixed Mode Survey Designs on SimpleandComplexAnalyses.London: CityUniversityofLondon. [39] Mayer-Schönberger, V., and Cukier, K. (2013): Big data: A Revolution That Will TransformHowWeLive,WorkandThink.NewYork: HoughtonMifflin. [40] Murray, D. E. (1988): The context of oral and written language: A framework for modeandmediumswitching.LanguageinSociety,17(3),351–373. [41] Ong, W. J. (1977): Interfaces of the World: Studies in the Evolution of Conscious- nessandCulture.London: CornellUniversityPress. [42] Roberts,C.(2007): MixingModesofDataCollectioninSurveys: AMethodological Review.London: ESRCNationalCentreforResearchMethods. [43] Roxas, B., and Lindsay, V. (2012): Social desirability bias in survey research on sustainable development in small firms: An exploratory analysis of survey mode effect.BusinessStrategyandtheEnvironment,21(4),223–235. [44] Sakshaug, J. W., Yan, T., and Tourangeau, R. (2010): Nonresponse error, measure- menterror,andmodeofdatacollection: Tradeoffsinamulti-modesurveyofsensi- tiveandnon-sensitiveitems.PublicOpinionQuarterly,74(5),907–933. [45] Sax, L., Gilmartin, S., and Bryant, A. (2003): Assessing response rates and nonre- sponse bias in Web and paper surveys. Research in Higher Education, 44(4), 409– 432. Survey Mode as a Moderator ... 17 [46] Schuman, H., and Presser, S. (1996): Questions and Answers in Attitude Surveys: ExperimentsonQuestionForm,WordingandContext.London: Sage. [47] Schwarz, N., and Strack, F. (1991): Context effects in attitude surveys: Applying cognitivetheorytosocialresearch.EuropeanReviewofSocialPsychology,31-50. [48] Schwarz, N., and Sudman, S. (1991): Context Effects in Social and Psychological Research.NewYork: Springer-Verlag. [49] Snijkers, G., Hox, J., and de Leeuw, E. (1995): The effect of computer-assisted interviewing on data quality. A review. International Journal of Market Research, 37(4),325–344. [50] Tourangeau, R., and Rasinski, K. (1988): Cognitive processes underlying context effectsinattitudemeasurement.PsychologicalBulletin,103(3),299–314. [51] Tourangeau, R., and Yan, T. (2007): Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychological Bulletin,133(5),859–883. [52] Tourangeau, R., Rips, L., and Rasinski, K. (2000): The Psychology of Survey Re- sponse.Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress. [53] Uhan,S.(1998): Pravainnepravamnenja.Ljubljana: Fakultetazadružbenevede. [54] Vannieuwunhuyze, J., Loosveldt, G., and Molenberghs, G. (2010): A method for evaluating mode effects in mixed-mode surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 74(5), 1027–1045.